New Tomographic Imaging Technique to Perceive Important Acoustic Cues for Bats

The ability of some bats to spot motionless prey in the dark has baffled experts until now. By creating the first visual images from echolocation, researchers reveal we have been missing how bats sense their world.

Elite hunter from bat world has previously unknown cues at its disposal. Credit: Elizabeth Clare

Publishing in the journal eLife, scientists from Queen Mary University of London (QMUL) and the University of Bristol suggest that instead of searching for prey directly, bats intimately learn the layout of a home range - down to the surface of individual leaves and stones. When one of these familiar leaves is covered by an insect, the normal echoes are interrupted and this "acoustic shadow" is a very strong signal to the hunting bat.

The researchers also describe a new phenomenon of "acoustic camouflage", employed by hunted prey to elude the elite hunters.

Most bats hunt by detecting the movement of flying insects using echolocation. Only a third of all bat species are able to hunt for prey on surfaces. Previous studies have concluded that these bats must detect prey by sight, smell and sound.

However, the common big-eared bat Micronycteris microtis, a neotropical species from South and Central America, has been found to use echolocation alone. Experts have speculated that by hovering beside prey, this species bombards it and the surface on which it rests with short sound waves. By continually changing their angle of scanning, the prey becomes easier to pick out.

In the new study, researchers in Bristol developed a new imaging technique to allow them to enter the bat's world view. They built four acoustic cues into the system, combining the duration and amplitude of echoes with the depth profile of objects and the strength of the shadow cast by insects.

"Using information like X-rays to build pictures is well established in medicine with the use of CT scans and ultrasound, but no-one has ever before attempted to create images from sounds generated in ecology," says lead author Dr Elizabeth Clare from QMUL.

"We constructed a brand new technique for tomographic imaging which allows us to perceive more accurately which cues are most important to the bat," says co-author Marc Holderied from the University of Bristol.

The research team found that the acoustic cues available to bats to detect motionless prey don't improve greatly if the profile of the insect seems to stand out more. It might be expected that a hawkmoth, which rests with its wings sticking up, would be easier to find than a small butterfly with extremely thin wings. Instead, detection improves according to the kind of surface on which the insect rests.

Insects are much easier to detect on flat surfaces - such as slate and smooth leaves - and hardest to detect on rough bark. The data help explain why some species of bat remain faithful to a defined hunting ground and why they scan leaves so thoroughly. But surprisingly, the team's data suggest that bat should not look for prey, but search surfaces with which they are familiar, particular smooth ones, and then home in on patches where there are 'echo gaps' in what they'd normally find.

"Our findings also suggest a new phenomenon of acoustic camouflage, where insects are harder to discern on rough surfaces such as bark, and bats compensate for this by focusing their attention on the simpler, mirror-like surfaces in their patch," says Dr Clare.

Source: http://www.elifesciences.org

Tell Us What You Think

Do you have a review, update or anything you would like to add to this news story?

Leave your feedback
Your comment type
Submit

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.